18 August 2021

Differences in impulsivity between individuals are linked to both experience and gene expression, according to a study on the ancestor of domestic chickens, the red junglefowl.

More impulsive individuals are more likely to respond rapidly to situations without planning or considering the consequences. In many species, including humans, impulsivity differs between individuals, but we do not yet understand why this is, as research into what lies behind these differences is limited.Hanne LøvlieHanne Løvlie Photo credit Anna Nilsen

“Variation in impulsivity is especially puzzling, because individuals with high impulsivity can suffer negative consequences, such as taking risks without considering the outcome. We expect natural selection over time to favour behaviour that benefits the individual, so why do we regularly observe individuals who are considerably more impulsive than others?” asks Hanne Løvlie, associate professor in the Department of Physics, Chemistry and Biology at Linköping University, who led the study that has been published in the journal Animal Behaviour.

The LiU researchers looked in more detail at how impulsivity could be influenced by underlying factors. They studied the red junglefowl (Gallus gallus), the ancestor of our domestic chickens and a frequently used species for studies on behavioural differences and cognition, measuring animal “intelligence”.red junglefowl chick looking at mealworm through transparent plastic tube.Junglefowl chick undergoing impulsivity testing. Photo credit Sam Hurenkamp

To investigate whether junglefowl chicks differed in impulsivity, the researchers used an already established test in which a reward (a mealworm) is placed inside a transparent tube (see video below). The impulsive response is to try to reach the reward directly through the solid, transparent side of the tube, even though this is not possible. To get the mealworm, a chick must instead curb its impulsivity and remember what it has previously learnt – that it can reach the reward from the open end of the tube. The researchers counted how many times each chick pecked at the transparent tube trying to get the reward, which is a measure of how impulsive they were. By repeating the experiment several times, the researchers also measured how well each chick learnt to reduce its impulsivity.

The scientists wanted to see how early experiences could influence impulsivity. Before testing how impulsive chicks were, they assigned each chick at random to one of three treatments. In one treatment, chicks received training which aimed to improve their cognitive abilities, resulting in ‘cognitively enriched’ chicks. Chicks in the second treatment were permitted to interact with the cognitive testing equipment, but were not trained themselves, and were thus ‘environmentally enriched’ (but not cognitively enriched). Chicks in the third treatment did not receive any enrichment while growing up.

The results showed that these differences in early experience did indeed affect impulsivity in the junglefowl chicks, but not in the manner that was expected.portait photo of Laura GarnhamLaura Garnham, PhD student Photo credit Lejla Kronbäck

“Intriguingly, cognitively enriched chicks, who had been trained to pass other cognitive tests, were more impulsive than the other chicks. This goes against our initial expectations but is compatible with our finding that environmentally enriched chicks were slower to learn to reduce impulsivity. Thus, all chicks that received enrichment were less able to curb their impulsivity. Our results support discoveries from earlier studies that suggest that enrichment can increase impulsivity, and highlight the potential role of cognitive enrichment”, says Laura Garnham, PhD student at LiU and one of the researchers behind the study.

Variation between individuals was also influenced by brain gene expression. The scientists investigated the levels of expression of several genes that are involved in two important brain signalling systems, the serotonin system and the dopamine system, which in other species have been linked to impulsive behaviour.red junglefowl chick.Junglefowl chick showing impulsive behaviour. Photo credit Sam Hurenkamp

“We found that impulsivity correlated with the expression of one gene linked to the signalling molecule serotonin, and two genes linked to the signalling molecule dopamine. This shows us that not only differences in experience, but also genetic factors can contribute to differences in impulsivity between individuals”, says Sara Ryding, who worked in the study while on an exchange visit from the University of Manchester. Sara is now a PhD student at Deakin University in Melbourne, Australia.

The research has received financial support from the FORMAS research council.

The article:Impulsivity is affected by cognitive enrichment and links to brain gene expression in red junglefowl chicks”, Sara Ryding, Laura C. Garnham, Robin N. Abbey-Lee, Irina Petkova, Anastasia Kreshchenko and Hanne Løvlie (2021), Animal Behaviour, vol 178, 195-207, published online 8 July 2021, doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2021.06.007

Translation by George Farrants

How do you test impulsivity in chicks?

Impulsivity test

Some individuals are more impulsive than others – even among animals. But why this is so has not yet been revealed. To learn more about differences in impulsivity and factors that affect them, researchers test how impulsive chicks are – with the help of mealworms.

For English captions: activate subtext in the Youtube window

Want to know more about the research?

More research on animal behaviour

Latest news from LiU

Portrait of a woman (Seetha Murty)

Two champions for children become honorary doctors

Indian principal Seetha Murty has worked with children’s and young people’s learning throughout her professional life. American professor Cindy W. Christian has devoted her career to developing the care of abused children.

Two researchers in a lab.

A single atom layer of gold – LiU researchers create goldene

For the first time, scientists have managed to create sheets of gold only a single atom layer thick. According to the researchers from LiU this has given the gold new properties that can make it suitable for use in various applications.

Researcher (Xenofon Strakosas) by a microscope.

Ten million donation for research position in electronic medicine

The Stig Wadström Foundation is donating around SEK ten million to LiU, to fund a research position in electronic medicine. The researcher chosen for this position is Xenophon Strakosas at the Laboratory of Organic Electronics in Norrköping.